Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) encompass a set of conditions characterised by impairments in social interactions, communication and the presence of repetitive patterns of behaviour and restricted interests1. With a prevalence of approximately 1 case per 100 children, ASD diagnosis shows an overall male to female ratio of 4:12. Sexual dimorphisms are seen in memory, cognitive flexibility, verbal fluency, and social communication, with studies in children and adolescents showing that females are more sociable and display better fine motor skills; while exhibiting greater difficulty internalising and expressing emotions and reduced verbal control3,4. On the other hand, male subjects display more significant repetitive and stereotyped behaviours and greater difficulties in social skills3,4.

According to the DSM-5 and other literature, higher severity of symptoms and higher number of comorbidities, such as epilepsy and intellectual disability (ID), are present in females diagnosed with ASD1,5,6. In the cases where both ASD and ID are identified, a more proportional gender ratio is observed, hinting towards a lower likelihood of ASD diagnosis for females with normal or borderline intellectual disability7. The sex differences observed in both the severity of the symptomatology and bias in diagnosis have been explored in the ‘imprinted x-liability threshold model’8, the ‘extreme male brain theory’9 and the ‘female protective effect theory’10 as some of the rationale proposed to explain genetic and neurobiological sex differences.

Sex-effects are seen in EphB2+/- mice where females, but not males, display memory deficits and increased repetitive behaviours11. Tsc2+/− females present alterations in social interactions and more simplified communication while performing social tasks12 and 16p11.2del/+ females mice display a sex-specific increase in susceptibility to anxiety-like phenotypes following stress13. Recently, a study on Nf1+/− mice showed a sexually dimorphic impact on hippocampal neurochemistry, but superior performance in social behaviours in females14. The Mthfr+/− mice display recognition memory impairments and anxiety-like behaviours in both males and females, although with different magnitudes15. In contrast, the Fmr1 knockout model has shown a lack of sex-specific effects, since both males and females present audiogenic seizures, hyperactivity, and deficits in memory16.

Maternal behaviours are sex-specific behaviours shown by dams during the perinatal period, which include aggression towards intruder males, pup licking and nursing, or pup retrieval to the nest. These behaviours can help reveal underlying dysfunction in innate social phenotypes. In this domain, for example, Shank2 knockout female mice have been reported to abandon newborn pups upon delivery17; surrogate Mecp2 heterozygous females show deficits in pup gathering behaviour18; and pups raised by Tsc2 mutant dams display alterations in vocalization19.

In this study, we used a model generated via the deletion of Gprasp2, a gene previously linked to ASD and ID20,21,22,23. The G-protein Coupled Receptor Associated Sorting Protein (GPRASP) family of proteins are known to regulate the trafficking of diverse GPCRs, such as, dopamine, muscarinic and glutamatergic metabotropic receptors24. We previously showed that GPRASP2 modulates synaptic maturation, hippocampal function, and behaviour. We found that deletion of Gprasp2 in male mice produced deficits in social behaviours, memory impairments and led to weight increases25.

Here, we show that Gprasp2 females replicate the deficits previously observed in Gprasp2 KO male mice in terms of memory, social interactions and weight alterations and additionally, display changes in social attachment and maternal care. We find that deletion of Gprasp2 leads to a significant impairment in maternal care during the post parturition phase, which impacts development at early stages.

Methods

Mice

Gprasp2 mutant mice generation and genotyping methods were previously described in detail in Edfawy et al.25. Mouse cages were maintained at a constant temperature (22 °C) and humidity (60%), under a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on from 7 am to 7 pm). Animals were allowed access to water and food ad libitum. Female animals, ages between 6 and 14 weeks, were used in the experiments performed in this study unless otherwise noted. Maintenance and handling of the animals was performed in compliance with all relevant ethical regulations for animal testing and research, including the guidelines of the Animals Use and Care Guidelines issued by FELASA and European Directives on Animal Welfare. All experiments were carried out under animal testing research protocols approved by ORBEA (Institutional Animal Welfare Body of the University of Coimbra/CNC, reference number 282/2021) and DGAV (Portuguese Regulatory Agency, reference number 8212/2021). Heterozygous (Gprasp2+/-) dams were bred with wild-type males (Gprasp2+/y) to produce hemizygous knockout males (Gprasp2-/y), wild-type males (Gprasp2+/y) and females (Gprasp2+/+) and heterozygous females (Gprasp2+/−). Heterozygous (Gprasp2+/−) dams were bred with knockout (Gprasp2-/y) males to produce hemizygous knockout (Gprasp2-/y) and wild-type males (Gprasp2+/y) along with heterozygous (Gprasp2+/-) and homozygous knockout females (Gprasp2-/-). All behavioural tests and quantifications were performed by trained experimentalists blinded to the animal genotype. The estrous cycle was determined for a subset of animals and no significant correlation was found between behaviour and cycle stage.

This study is reported in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.

Open field

The test was performed as previously described in Edfawy et al.25. Open field consisted of an opaque arena (40 × 40 × 30 cm) and mice were automatically video tracked using Ethovision XT (Noldus, Netherlands). Mice were placed at the corner of the apparatus and locomotor behaviour was recorded for 1 h. Indirect and homogeneous illumination of the room was provided by white LED lamps at 100 lx on the centre of the maze. Time spent in the centre zone (15 cm × 15 cm) and total distance travelled were evaluated using Ethovision XT (Noldus, Netherlands).

Elevated plus maze

The elevated plus maze consisted of an arena composed of four arms (30 × 5 cm), two of them open, the other two closed (walls 15 cm high). The animals were placed in the arena and left to freely roam for 10 min. On the two open arms, besides playing with the natural aversion mice show for open and elevated places, brightness is also added as an anxiogenic factor, with illumination of 100 lx. Mice were tracked using Ethovision XT (Noldus, Netherlands) and time spent on the open arms was evaluated.

T-maze test

The test was performed as previously described in Edfawy et al.25. Briefly, the apparatus consisted of a T-shaped maze (72 × 10 cm, TS0701-M, OpenScience) elevated from the floor (60 cm). Fresh bedding was added to the maze before testing the animals. The mice were placed in the initial part of the stem and allowed to explore the maze. After the animal entered one of the arms, a sliding door was placed in the initial part of the arm chosen, allowing the mouse to explore the chosen arm. After a 30 s retention period, the animal was gently removed and returned to the homecage. After another 30 s period, the animal was returned to the start arm and a second run was initiated. Directions of choice were recorded for each mouse and the percentage of alternation obtained. Five trials (two runs each) were conducted in the space of two consecutive days (three on the first day and two on the second). The floor in the maze was homogeneously illuminated at 15–20 lx.

Three-chamber social interaction test

The three-chamber arena was from Stoelting (Stoelting, Ireland). Mice were tested for voluntary social interaction as previously described26. The assay consisted of three sessions: the first session began with a 20-min habituation period during which the subject mouse freely explored all three chambers; next, the mouse was confined to the centre chamber and an empty wire cage (Empty) and a cage with an unfamiliar mouse (WT stimulus, Social Partner) were introduced to the side-chambers; in the second session, the subject mouse was then allowed to freely explore all three chambers for 20 min. Before the third and last session, the subject mouse was gently guided to the centre chamber while the empty wire cage was replaced with a caged WT stimulus mouse (Novel Partner). In the last session, the subject mouse was then left to explore all three chambers for 10 min. Stimulus mice were age-matched wild-type females previously habituated to the wire cages. The positions of the empty cage and “Social Partner” were alternated between tests. No position bias was observed. Time spent in close proximity was calculated using the automated software Ethovison XT (Noldus, Netherland). Preference index was calculated as the time spent in close proximity with the Novel/Social Partner divided by the sum of the time spent in close proximity to the Novel and Familiar/Empty partner.

Dyadic social interaction test

Mice were tested for reciprocal social interaction, as previously described26. Age-, sex- and weight-matched C57BL/6 females unfamiliar with the tested mice were used as stimulus partners. The test was performed in an open arena (40 × 40 × 30 cm) filled with fresh bedding. Illumination on the arena floor was kept at 30 lx during the test and the chamber was cleaned with 70% ethanol between trials. The target mouse was removed from the homecage and placed on one side of the chamber and separated by a solid partition from the matched partner. After the 10-min acclimatisation period, the barrier was removed, and social interactions were recorded for 30 min. Social interactions (sniffing, allogrooming, chasing) were quantified, being a second run-through performed where the interactions initiated by the tested animal were identified and differentiated. These behaviours were scored manually by observers blinded to the genotype of the animals using the Observer XT 9 software (Noldus, Netherland).

Marble burying test

The animals were placed in a new standard homecage (20 × 26 × 13 cm) filled with corn bedding up to 5 cm in height. 24 marbles were orderly and equally distanced on top of the bedding. The animals were placed in the cage and left to freely roam for 30 min. The number of marbles that were more than 25% visible were counted as unburied by at least three different observers blinded to the experimental conditions.

Nest building test

Mice were individually housed in an unfamiliar homecage (20 × 26 × 13 cm) with corn bedding and without environmental enrichment. In each cage, a single nestlet was added at 5 pm. Nest quality produced after 16 h was recorded for each mouse and assessed by at least three observers blinded to the experimental conditions following a 5-point rating scale as described in27.

Pup retrieval test

The retrieval assay was performed in the homecage of the dam. From PND2 to PND4, identical sessions were performed daily. On each test day, the pups were separated from the dam for 30 min prior to the test and kept warm during the separation, maintaining the nest untouched inside the cage. Immediately prior to the assay the dams were briefly removed from the homecage and three of the pups were placed in each corner of the cage except for the one closest to the nest. The dam was then reintroduced to the cage, being placed in the nest facing the wall. The latency for the female to retrieve all three pups to the nest was assessed. If after 10 min there would be pups still out of the nest, the test would be stopped, the remaining pups were retrieved and placed in the nest. In these instances, the latency score attributed was 600 s. All females assessed were primiparous females, having no previous gestation and maternal care experience.

Maternal aggression test

The test was performed on postnatal day 5 in the homecage of the dam. The pups were removed from their cage, 3 min before the start of the test to prevent infanticide, but the nest was left in place. For the test, sexually naïve C57BL/6 males were introduced to the cage and the interactions were recorded for 10 min. The latency to first attack, the number of aggressive bouts and the duration of these behaviours were assessed. Chasing, biting, and wrestling were considered aggressive behaviours/ attacks. These behaviours were scored manually by observers blinded to the genotype of the animals using the Observer XT 9 software (Noldus, Netherland). All females assessed were primiparous females, having no previous gestation and maternal care experience.

Ultrasonic vocalisations recordings

Isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs) were recorded from PND2 to PND15. USVs were recorded for 6 min in a sound-attenuated styrofoam box. The microphone was placed through a hole in the middle of the cover of the styrofoam box, about 6 cm above the pup which was placed in a small petri dish. Recordings were obtained using the UltraVox XT system (Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands), which could record the full spectrum of sound with a maximum frequency of 160 kHz. After recording USVs, pups were returned to the homecage. Between PND0 and PND1, the pups were marked on the paws to ensure that each pup was only recorded once daily.

All the USV recordings were analysed offline with the Ultravox software using a 250 kHz sampling rate and a maximum frequency of 120 kHz. The USVs were analysed manually by a researcher blind to the phenotype. The USVs were then distinguished manually within 10 different subtypes based on their qualitative and quantitative characteristics, according to the categories established by28. “Short” calls had a length of less than 5 ms; “Flat” calls had inflections of less than 3 kHz; “Downward” calls showed a continuous decrease in pitch; “Upward” calls showed a continuous increase in pitch; “Chevron” calls presented an inverted U-shape with an increase in frequency followed by a decrease; the “Complex” class exhibited one call containing two or more directional changes in frequency; Two-syllable calls displayed two components: a main call (flat or downward) and an additional short call with higher frequency in the end; “Composite” calls were composed of two calls, at different frequencies, emitted simultaneously; Frequency steps presented instantaneous frequency steps showing a vertical discontinuous step on a spectrogram with no interruption in time; Harmonics presented one main call but with additional calls of different frequencies surrounding it.

Cross fostering experiments

To carry out cross fostering experiments, programmed mating sessions were performed and litters born within 12 h of each other, originated from different genotyped females, were used. At PND0, the size and gender prevalence within the litters was assessed. Before switching the pups between Gprasp2+/+ and Gprasp2−/− dams, litters were culled to set the number of pups within each litter and the gender of pups between the two litters being tested. To minimise rejection from the dams, pups were maintained in heating pads during this process and placed in bedding from the foster dam prior to being placed in a novel cage. At PND2, PND8 and PND15 the pups were subjected to the ultrasonic vocalisation recording protocol, as described above. The pups were also handled and weighed daily.

Tissue collection

Animals were anaesthetised using isoflurane (IsoVet) and euthanized by decapitation. The different tissues (specified in Table 1) were collected and dissected on ice and immediately stored at – 80 °C, until further processing.

Table 1 Characteristics of the different tissues collected along with their purpose.

Biochemical analysis

For protein extraction, RIPA Lysis buffer (150 mM NaCl, 1% Triton X-100, 0.5% DOC, 0.1% SDS, 50 mM Tris pH 7.4) was added to the samples. Manual homogenization was performed until no clear tissue was visible followed by a sonication protocol (15 secs on, 30 secs off, 3 times) always keeping the samples refrigerated. Samples were then centrifuged at max rotation (g) for 20 min at 4 °C (Heraeus Fresco 21 Centrifuge, ThermoFisher). Protein quantification was performed using the BCA protein assay kit (Pierce BCA protein assay Kit, Thermo Scientific), following the manufacturer’s instructions. Protein samples (55 μg) were denatured with 4 × Laemmli sample buffer (Bio-Rad) and 10% β-mercaptoethanol. Samples were incubated at 95 °C for 5 min before western blotting.

Western blotting

Equal amounts of proteins were resolved by SDS-PAGE in 10% polyacrylamide gels. Proteins were transferred to PVDF membranes Immobilon-P (Millipore), blocked at room temperature (RT) for 1 h in 1 × TBS + 0.1% Tween-20 (TBS-T) with 5% milk. Membranes were incubated with primary antibodies (diluted in TBS-T with 0,5% milk) at 4 °C overnight before washing in TBS-T, 3 times for 15 min each, and incubated with secondary antibodies (1:10,000 anti-mouse, anti-rabbit, Jackson ImmunoResearch) for 120 min at room temperature. Following three-time washes in TBS-T, the membranes were incubated with ECF western blot substrate (Vistra ECF, Sigma-Aldrich).

Signal was visualised on ChemiDoc Imaging System (Biorad). Antibodies used in western blotting experiments were the following: anti-GPRASP2 (12,159–1-AP, 1:1000; Proteintech). For all experiments, anti-β-actin (A5441, 1:10,000; Sigma-Aldrich) antibody was used as loading control.

qRT-PCR

Total RNA was extracted from hypothalamus collected from both nulliparous (4 months old) and primiparous females (4 months old, 15 days post-delivery, before weaning) with the use of the NucleoSpin RNA kit and according to the instructions of the supplier (Macherey–Nagel). RNA extracted (1.15 μg) was retrotranscribed to complementary DNA using the NZY First-Strand cDNA Synthesis kit (NZYTech), following the manufacturer’s instructions. The synthesised complementary DNA was diluted 15 × in RNAse-free water and stored at − 20 °C. To avoid genomic DNA contamination a DNase I treatment step was performed during the RNA extraction and the primers’ design (specified in Table 2) targeted regions between adjacent exons, when possible. Primer specificity was verified by the melting curve shape, peak temperature, and the amplification of a single product with the expected size confirmed by electrophoresis in an agarose gel.

Table 2 Primer sequences used for qRT-PCR reactions.

In the case of the oxytocin receptor, an extra step was added given the low concentration values of this product. For this, a pre-amplification step was added. This step consisted of the following PCR protocol: denaturation step for 2 min at 95 °C followed by 14 cycles of amplification (10 s at 95 °C and 4 min at 60 °C). For this, a 50 μL reaction mix containing 5 units of MyTaq DNA Polymerase (Bioline), 0.015 mM of specific primers (forward and reverse primers for both OxtR and Hprt), 10 μL of 5 × MyTaq Reaction buffer (Bioline), 2 μL of the previously transcribed cDNA in RNAse free water. The resulting product was then used in the following PCR reaction.

For the final PCR reaction, a mix containing 5 μL of NZYSpeedy qPCR Green Master Mix (NZYTech), 1 μL of forward and reverse primer mix (10 μM each) and 4 μL of complementary DNA was added to each well. The reaction was initiated with a polymerase activation step for 30 s at 95 °C. After, 40 cycles of a 5 s denaturation step at 95 °C followed by 20 s of an annealing/extension step at the optimal primer annealing temperature of 60 °C were performed. After 40 cycles of amplification, a melting curve protocol was performed. All reactions were performed in duplicates. Quantitative PCR assays were performed and analysed with a real-time PCR system (StepOnePlus, Applied Biosystems, ThermoFisher Scientific, USA). The comparative CT method (DDCT) was used to quantify the relative gene expression of each sample, using Hprt as the endogenous reference gene. The endogenous reference sample used corresponded to the Gprasp2+/+ nulliparous females.

Mammary gland whole-mount staining

Abdominal mammary glands were collected and placed on a gelatinated microscope slide. The glands were manipulated and spread using blunt tweezers and immediately placed in Kahle’s fix solution overnight. The glands were then washed in 70% ethanol for 15 min, gradually changed and rinsed with distilled water for 5 min. They were then stained overnight in a carmine alum staining solution. The glands were afterwards washed for 15 min in 70% ethanol, 95% ethanol and 100% ethanol in order. The glands were then cleared in xylene for 72 h (or until the fat pads appeared transparent) at which point they were mounted using Richard-Allan scientific mounting medium (Thermo Scientific). Slides were stored at room temperature until further analysis. Whole mammary gland images were acquired in an Axio Imager Z2 microscope (Zeiss, Germany) with an EC Plan Neofluor 10x/0,3 lens. Each image consisted in a compilation of tiles encompassing the whole tissue. A crop of 1500 × 1500 pixels in size was utilised as a representation of the whole tissue. Tracing reconstruction was performed using the NeuronJ plugin in ImageJ, extracting the total length of the vasculature in each crop. Both abdominal mammary glands were analysed, and a mean of both sides was used when plotting the data. Experiments were performed blind to the animal’s genotype both during image acquisition and analysis.

Statistical analysis

Data is represented as mean values ± SEM. Statistical analysis was performed using parametric (one sample t-test) and non-parametric tests (two-tailed Mann–Whitney test, two-way repeated measures ANOVA), followed by post hoc analysis when appropriate, as indicated in figure legends. Analysis was performed using Graphpad (Prism) or MATLAB (Mathworks). Subject randomisation was performed. Sample size calculation were performed using G*Power and considering the authors prior experience. Statistical significance was defined as ****p < 0.0001, ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *,#p < 0.05.

Ethics approval

All relevant ethical approvals were collected and include ORBEA (Institutional Animal Welfare Body of the University of Coimbra/CNC, reference number 282/2021) and DGAV (Portuguese Regulatory Agency, reference number 8212/2021).

Results

We started by performing a western blot analysis to confirm gene-dose dependence of protein expression in females of all genotypes (Fig. 1a). As expected, heterozygous females for Gprasp2 show an overall decrease of approximately 50% in the expression of the protein in 2-month-old whole brain tissue samples (Fig. 1a). When assessing the expression of GPRASP2 in the brain, we observe this protein is highly enriched in the hypothalamus in both wild-type and heterozygous females (Fig. 1b). In terms of overall morphology, Gprasp2−/− (KO) and Gprasp2+/− (HET) females did not display gross abnormalities except for an increase in body weight in KO females, significant after 4-months of age, and increasingly more striking with age (F = 10.94, p = 0.0001, Fig. 1c and d). In this work, we studied female Gprasp2+/+, Gprasp2+/− and Gprasp2−/− during the juvenile period (6–14 weeks of age), before significant differences in body weight were observed that could interfere with other analyses.

Figure 1
figure 1

Gpraps2 mutant female mice display increased body weight and deficits in memory and social behaviours. (a) Western blotting of 2-month-old whole brain samples shows decreased expression of GPRASP2 in heterozygous females and protein deletion in Gprasp2-/- when compared with wild-type littermates, WT n = 7, HET n = 7, KO n = 3, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (b) GPRASP2 is highly enriched in the hypothalamus of 1 month-old Gprasp2+/+ and Gprasp2+/- females, n = 7, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (c) Representative images from 1 year old WT, HET, and KO female mice (scale bar represents 1 cm). (d) Gprasp2-/- females present an increase in body weight starting approximately at 4-months of age becoming more striking throughout adulthood; WT n = 18, HET n = 23, KO n = 13, two-way repeated measures ANOVA. (ei) Gprasp2 deficient females present memory and social impairments. (e) Gprasp2 deficient females show no significant alterations in the open field test; WT n = 14, HET n = 13, KO n = 9, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (f) In the t-maze for spontaneous alternation, Gprasp2-/- and Gprasp2+/- females display impaired alternation when compared with control mice; WT n = 17, HET n = 14, KO n = 12; One sample t test against hypothetical value of chance alternation at 50%. (g) Gprasp2-/- and Gprasp2+/- females show no impairments in the sociability component of the three-chamber social test; WT n = 17, HET n = 14, KO n = 12; One sample t test against hypothetical value of chance preference at 0.5. (h) In the social novelty component of the three-chamber test, Gprasp2-/- females display reduced preference index for a novel social partner when comparing to WT controls; WT n = 17, HET n = 14, KO n = 12; One sample t test against hypothetical value of chance preference at 0.5. (i) Gprasp2+/- and Gprasp2-/- females show a slight and significant, respectively, increase in the percentage of marbles unburied in the marble burying test; WT n = 17, HET n = 14, KO n = 12, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Statistical significance: *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001 (CER-cerebellum; CTX-cortex; STR-striatum; HIPP-hippocampus; HYP-hypothalamus).

Given the previously described mutations in Gprasp2 associated with ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders20,21,22,23 and the behavioural alterations previously described in Gprasp2 KO males25, we assessed if females also presented behavioural alterations in a battery of tests. Initially, an assessment of anxiety-like levels was performed through an open field test (WT: 339.9 ± 58.83 s; HET: 372.5 ± 53.2 s; KO: 382.1 ± 70.75 s; Fig. 1e) and elevated plus maze (WT: 46.4 ± 6.14 s; HET: 25.2 ± 6.27 s; KO: 39.1 ± 8.84 s; Supplementary Fig. 1a). In both behavioural paradigms, no significant alterations were observed between KO or HET females with WT controls (p > 0.05). Additionally, no gross abnormalities were found in motor function when compared total distance travelled in the open field (WT: 11,839 ± 723.9 cm; HET: 11,195 ± 739.7 cm; KO: 11,486 ± 1298 cm; P > 0.05, Supplementary Fig. 1b). Analysis of possible alterations in working memory using the T-maze test for spontaneous alternation, showed that both heterozygous and knockout females present significant deficits in memory (WT: 65.88 ± 5.08%, t(16) = 3.128, p = 0.0065; HET: 60.00 ± 6.63%, t(13) = 1.508, p = 0.1554; KO: 51.67 ± 5.75, t(11) = 0.2898, p = 0.7774; Fig. 1f). To assess for alterations in social behaviours and interactions, the three-chamber social test and the social dyadic paradigm were performed. In the three-chamber test, no major differences were observed in the social preference part of the test, as all genotypes significantly showed a preference index above 50% (WT: p < 0.001; HET: p < 0.05; KO: p < 0.01; Fig. 1g). However, deficits were observed in the second part of the test which assesses the response of the animals to social novelty. Here, Gprasp2 KO females showed no preference between a novel partner and a familiar one (WT: p < 0.05; HET: p < 0.001; KO: p > 0.05; Fig. 1h). In the social dyadic test, HET and KO females show a trend towards increased interactions with a stimulus, but no significant differences were found when compared to WT control females (pWT × HET > 0.05; pWT × KO > 0.05; Supplementary Fig. 1c). In the marble burying test, an assay that often appears altered in various animal models of ASD, Gprasp2-/- females presented an increase in the percentage of marbles unburied when compared to WT controls (WT: 32.59 ± 4.001; HET: 38.99 ± 4.763, pWT × HET = 0.2031; KO: 45.14 ± 3.305, pWT × KO = 0.0061; Fig. 1i). Together, this behavioural characterization suggests that Gprasp2 KO females display a significant impairment in assays requiring memory and discrimination of social novelty. A comparison between the phenotypes observed in Gprasp2-KO females and our previous report on Gprasp2 KO males25 is reported thoroughly in Supplementary Table 1. Most salient changes include an effect of Gprasp2 deletion on body weight increase and working memory and social memory impairments, regardless of the sex of the animals.

Given the impact of social bond between dams and their offspring in early development, we decided to focus on the effect of Gprasp2 deletion on maternal care and the consequence to their offspring. To achieve this, females of the three genotypes were crossed with C57BL/6 males as illustrated in Fig. 2a, and their behaviours after parturition were explored. We found no gross alterations in nest building performance and fertility (Fig. 2b and c). However, while no significant changes were observed in the number of pups present at PND0 (postnatal day 0, day of delivery) (Fig. 2c), a significantly lower number of pups from Gprasp2-/- dams survived until PND15 (Fig. 2d). The percentage of pup survival was close to 90% for the litters of primiparous WT and HET dams. However, litters from primiparous KO dams showed a significantly lower percentage, with only 60% of the pups surviving beyond PND2 (Fig. 2e). Looking into the body weight of the pups throughout early development, a small decrease in pups from KO dams was seen at PND5, however, these changes were no longer present at PND15 (WTPND5: 2.566 ± 0.04872 g; KOPND5: 2.458 ± 0.05425 g; p < 0.05; WTPND15: 6.298 ± 0.08329 g; KOPND15: 6.271 ± 0.1187 g; p > 0.05, Fig. 2f). These data, however, contrasts with the significant weight gain seen in adult, male or female, GPRASP2 mutant mice. To further explore the drop in pup survival at such an early stage, we performed an analysis of mammary gland morphology. We found that prior to birth, Gprasp2 KO females already presented a significant decrease in the length of the ducts in the mammary gland (WT: 113.3 ± 2.046 mm; HET: 102.5 ± 4.83 mm; KO: 97.14 ± 4.856 mm, pWT x KO = 0.029; Fig. 2g). Given that milk production could be impaired not only by constraints in gland physiology, but also considering the role of oxytocin receptors in this process, we performed quantitative PCR analysis of hypothalamic samples collected from both nulliparous and primiparous females (before weaning) to assess possible alterations in the expression of oxytocin and its receptor. Using qPCR analysis in hypothalamic samples, we found that Gprasp2-/- nulliparous females presented a significant decrease in the OxtR levels when compared to WT controls (WT: 1.288 ± 0.4673; HET: 0.4998 ± 0.0946; KO: 0.2285 ± 0.09462, pWT × KO = 0.0317; Fig. 2h). Regarding the mRNA levels for oxytocin, we found a slight increase between primiparous females when compared to nulliparous, but no changes between genotypes (pWT-NP × HET-NP > 0.05; pWT-NP × KO-NP > 0.05; Fig. 2i). Besides the expected decrease in Gprasp2 mRNA expression, we also found a trend for increased Gprasp2 expression following parturition in both wild-type and heterozygous females (Fig. 2j).

Figure 2
figure 2

Decreased litter survival and deficits in reproductive system in Gprasp2 mutant dams. (a) Schematic diagram of the matings established and possible genotypes of each litter depending on the female progenitor. (b) Females show no significant alterations in the nest building activity; WT n = 17, HET n = 14, KO n = 12, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (c) At PND0, no significant differences are seen in terms of the size of the litters from the Gprasp2 deficient females when compared to WT controls, WT n = 12, HET n = 9, KO n = 8, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (d) Pups born from Gprasp2-/- females have a lower chance of survival when compared to pups born from WT controls, WT n = 12, HET n = 9, KO n = 8, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (e) Majority of mortality of pups from Gprasp2-/- females occurs during the two first postnatal days, WT n = 12, HET n = 9, KO n = 8. (f) Weights in the pups nurtured by dams of different genotypes throughout early development, show a small reduction at P5 in animals from KO dams, that is recovered by P15; (+ /y) Gprasp2 +/+dams n = 38; (+ / +) Gprasp2 +/+dams n = 42; (+ /y) Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 21; (-/y)Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 16; (+ / +) Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 28; (+ /-) Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 21; (-/y)Gprasp2 -/-dams n = 47; (+ /-) Gprasp2 -/-dams n = 40, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (g) Mammary gland duct complexity is significantly lower in glands from Gprasp2-/- females (representative images scale bar 5 mm) WT n = 5, HET n = 7, KO n = 9, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (h) Oxytocin receptors in the hypothalamus show decreased mRNA expression in both KO and HET nulliparous (NP) and primiparous (PP) females when compared to their WT controls, n = 5/group, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (i) No significant alterations are observed between genotypes in terms of mRNA levels of oxytocin in the hypothalamus of these females, being a tendency for increase present in all genotypes when comparing PP to NP females, n = 5/group, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (j) Gprasp2 seems to be slightly increased in PP females when compared to NP females, n = 5/group, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Statistical significance: *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01.

We then studied if maternal care provided by the dams was impaired in the pup retrieval test. Here, Gprasp2-/- dams presented a significant increase in the latency to retrieve the first pup (F = 3.796, p = 0.0257) and the third pup in all the three trials (F = 3.633, p = 0.03), but no significant changes in the retrieval of the second pup (F(2, 102) = 1.151, p > 0.05), when compared to WT dams (Fig. 3a-c). Overall, normalising the values to latency to the first trial, we observe that both KO and HET dams presented a higher latency to retrieve pups in all three trials, and KO dams exhibited a significantly higher latency when compared to WT controls (F = 7.267, p = 0.0008; Fig. 3d). During the peripartum period, female mice display innate protective behaviours and aggression particularly towards male intruders29, which can be assessed with the maternal aggression test (Fig. 3e-h). However, we found no significant differences between dams of either genotypes in either latency to attack (Fig. 3f), total number of attacks (Fig. 3g) or number of anogenital bites (Fig. 3h). Together, these data show that Gprasp2 knockout dams display an alteration in pup-directed behaviours, but not in aggressive responses towards intruders.

Figure 3
figure 3

Gprasp2 knockout impairs maternal behaviour. (a-d) Gprasp2-/- females show a significant increase in latency in retrieving the first (a) and third pup (c), being this increased latency still seen although not as strikingly for the second pup (b). Overall, Gprasp2-/- females show an increased latency to retrieve pups when compared to WT controls (d) data normalised to Gprasp2+/+ females, trial 1 relative to each pup), WT n = 12, HET n = 11, KO n = 14, two-way repeated measures ANOVA. (e) Schematic diagram of some of the behaviours analysed in the maternal aggression test. (f–h) Gprasp2-/- females show a tendency for increased aggression with a slight decrease in latency to attack the male intruder (f) with no difference in terms of total number of attacks (g) and slight increase in number of anogenital bites (h), two-tailed Mann–Whitney test WT n = 13, HET n = 13, KO n = 12. All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Statistical significance: *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01.

In the case of rodents, ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs) are important for communication between the dams and the pups in early stages of development30,31,32. USVs can trigger maternal care and facilitate communication between the mother and their offspring31,33. To better understand this, we analysed USV emissions from isolated pups, at three different stages of development, PND2, PND8 and PND15. At PND2, pups from KO and HET dams, regardless of their genotypes, showed a significant decrease in the number of calls emitted when compared to pups from WT-control dams (WT: 300.1 ± 27.73; HET: 212.5 ± 18.14, pWT × HET = 0.0213; KO: 102.5 ± 26.81, pWT × KO < 0.0001; Fig. 4a). When looking into these differences but discriminating the genotype of the pups within the litters, we observed that the number of USVs emitted by pups from Gprasp2 KO dams, was lower when compared to wild-type pups from both WT and HET dams (Fig. 4b). Although not statistically significant, a decrease was also observed in pups from Gprasp2 KO dams when compared to genotype-matched pups from HET dams, and a similar effect with genotype-matched pups from HET dams to WT dams (Fig. 4b). This could suggest an effect on pup vocalisation that is dependent on the genotype of the pup and the genotype of the dam.

Figure 4
figure 4

Reduced number and complexity of calls in male pups reared by knockout dams. (a) At PND2, pups from Gprasp2-/- and Gprasp2+/- dams show a significant decrease in overall vocalisations, WT n = 4 dams, HET n = 6 dams, KO n = 4 dams, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (b) The decrease is present in both males (-/y) and females (+ /-) at this stage when compared with calls from wild-type pups from Gprasp2+/+ and Gprasp2+/- females, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (c) Examples of the different categories within which the calls were placed and their two broad groups (simple and complex). (d) When looking into the characteristics of these vocalisations, a decrease in terms of the complexity of the vocalisations is observed, at PND2, in pups from Gprasp2-/- and Gprasp2+/- dams, especially in the Gprasp2-/y males. (e) Unravelling the characteristics of these calls even further, a higher predominance of downward calls can be observed at PND2 in the males from Gprasp2-/- dams when compared to WT controls. (+ /y)WT dams n = 15; (+ / +)WT dams n = 12; (+ /y)Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 13; (-/y)Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 5; (+ / +)Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 14; (+ /-)Gprasp2 +/-dams n = 9; (-/y)Gprasp2 -/-dams n = 12; (+ /-)Gprasp2 -/-dams n = 7, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Statistical significance: *p < 0.05 and ****p < 0.0001.

Next, USVs were categorised according to their complexity as represented in Fig. 4c. A broad analysis of complexity of the calls showed a significant decrease in the percentage of complex calls in male Gprasp2-/y pups from HET and KO dams when compared to male Gprasp2+/y pups from WT dams ((+ /y)WT: 51.45 ± 2.408%; (−/y)HET: 36.79 ± 7.332%, p(+/y) WT × (-/y) HET = 0.0328; (-/y)KO: 24.17 ± 6.732%, p(+/y) WT x (-/y) KO = 0.0025; Fig. 4d). However, we found no alteration in call complexity in females from either WT, HET or KO dams (Fig. 4d). Using a subdivision of USVs into ten categories previously defined by Scattoni et al.28, we could observe an increase in the percentage of simple calls, especially downward calls, emitted by Gprasp2-/y pups from both HET (54.54 ± 21.11%, p = 0.0655) and KO dams (63.96 ± 21.45%, p > 0.001) when compared to WT controls (37.97 ± 7.19%) (Fig. 4e). At PND8, no significant differences were observed in the number of calls between the offspring of WT and KO dams although a significant increase is seen in the number of calls from offspring of HET dams (Supplementary Fig. 2a). Regarding the complexity of these calls, a male-specific decrease is observed in Gprasp2+/y pups from HET dams and Gprasp2-/y pups from KO dams when compared to Gprasp2+/y pups from WT dams ((+ /y)WT: 62.52 ± 2.958%; (+ /y)HET: 50.48 ± 5.605%, P(+/y) WT x (+/y) HET = 0.0292; (-/y)KO: 49.85 ± 5.67%, P(+/y) WT x (-/y) KO = 0.1028; Supplementary Fig. 2b-c). At PND15, no significant differences were observed in the number or complexity of USVs (Supplementary Fig. 2a-c).

To disentangle the effect of dam-genotype and pup-genotype interaction in the USV number and complexity, we decided to perform a cross-fostering experiment. To do so, litters from Gprasp2 WT and Gprasp2 KO dams were switched at PND0, as illustrated in Fig. 5a. Daily weighting of the pups revealed that litters fostered by WT dams showed an increase in body weight at PND15, when compared to WT pups fostered by KO dams (KOfostered pups: 6.599 ± 0.2076 g; WTfostered pups: 7.180 ± 0.1833; p < 0.05; Fig. 5b). When contrasted to the weight in the normal breedings (Fig. 2f), and the weight gain in adult GPRASP2 mutant mice, this data suggests that weight gain in mutant pups is limited when the nursing dam is performed by a KO dam.

Figure 5
figure 5

Cross-fostering by wildtype dams rescues neurodevelopmental trajectory in mutant male mice. (a) Schematic representation of the cross-fostering experiment. (b) Gprasp2-/y pups fostered by Gprasp2+/+ dams show an increase in body weight at PND15 when compared to wild-type males fostered by Gprasp2-/- dams, while no differences are observed at PND5, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (c) Cross-fostering led to a normalisation of the number of calls at PND2, the pups showing a similar number of calls with both fosters, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (d) The cross-fostering effect in the normalisation of the number of calls is not sex-specific with both males and females from both genotypes showing alterations, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (e) In terms of call complexity, at PND2, the significant differences previously observed with the normal crossing are also lost, the pups showing similar percentages when cared for by the fosters, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. (f) As for call characterization, no significant alterations are observed between pups independently of sex and foster genotype (+ /y) n = 8; (+ / +) n = 12; (-/y) n = 8; (+ /-) n = 12, two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Statistical significance: *p < 0.05.

When we analysed the effects of the cross fostering in USVs emitted by isolated pups, at PND2, we found the number of calls to be similar when assessing the data by the genotype of the foster dam (Fig. 5c). Moreover, when further analysing the genotype of the pups within the litters, no changes were observed in USV calls between WT and KO male pups when care was given by dams of opposing genotype ((+ /y)KO: 224.8 ± 30.65; (-/y)WT: 207.8 ± 26.72, p(+/y) KO x (-/y) WT > 0.05; Fig. 5d). At this stage of development, the number of calls emitted by KO pups fostered by WT dams is increased and the opposite occurred for pups fostered by KO dams, when comparing the absolute values from fostered pups with the same pups cared for by their biological dams (((+ /y)WT: 321.7 ± 34.00, p(+/y) WT × (+/y) KO = 0.1536; (-/y)KO: 104.3 ± 34.48, p(-/y) WT × (-/y) KO = 0.0691; see Fig. 4a and 5c). When comparing the complexity of calls at PND2, we can observe that the cross-fostering experiment rescued the alterations in complexity of the calls previously observed in KO male pups (Fig. 5e-f). At both PND8 and PND15 no significant changes were noted between vocalisations from pups fostered by both WT or KO dams (Supplementary Fig. 3). These results suggest that the effects observed with the “normal crossings'' are a combination of GxE effects dependent on the genotype of the pups and of the dam. This data suggests that while WT dams can act as a positive influence on the early development of the pups, the care provided by mutant dams can perturb early neurodevelopment, limit the weight gain in Gprasp2 mutant pups and perturb USV number and complexity.

Discussion

Behavioural alterations stemming from genetic disorders may impinge on progeny development, however, little is known on how specific gene mutation affect maternal care. The ability to form and maintain social bonds is a hallmark of social behaviour in mammals and deficits in these processes are a common hallmark in ASD34,35. The bonds formed during the perinatal period, particularly the formation of a mother-infant bond, strongly impact social behaviours at later stages of development, making this early postnatal phase a window of vulnerability for future impairments36,37, or when taken to the extreme in animals, maternal neglect will result in death.

Poor offspring survival is often paired with either impairments in maternal care, deficits in mammary gland function/ milk production or both38,39. In the Gprasp2 KO dams, we observed a combination of impairments in both maternal nursing behaviours, mammary gland development and OxtR gene expression. Since we did not observe dam aggression, and since the surviving pups developed normal body weight despite the significant decrease in litter size, the most parsimonious explanation for the neonatal death would be deficient milk production. In the case of Gprasp2-/- females, they can produce and eject milk, as verified by the presence of milk spots, but more subtle disruptions cannot be excluded. Studies have reported deficits in maternal behaviour in OxtR KO females postpartum, such as longer latencies to retrieve pups40. Conditional models of this deletion, where OxtR is only absent in the forebrain, report no striking alterations in maternal behaviours, but a significant increase in pup mortality38. Oxytocin is a non-peptide hormone, known for its role in sociosexual behaviours and female reproduction, with an emphasis in parturition and lactation, acting through oxytocin receptors. The expression of these Gq-coupled GPCRs is reported to increase significantly during pregnancy in the brain, particularly in the hypothalamic region41, and are thought to support—along with oxytocin, the onset and maintenance of maternal behaviours42,43. Altered levels of these receptors have also been reported to lead to an abnormal development of mammary glands in mice, with overall overexpression of OXTR leading to advanced maturation, hyperplasia, ductal distention and early involution of mammary glands in lactation44,45. The alteration observed in OxtR expression levels in primiparous Gprasp2 KO dams, are in line with the impairments observed in maternal care, mammary gland development and subsequent poor offspring survival.

As the postnatal period is considered a “vulnerability window” susceptible to environmental factors37, maternal care and maternal bond become an important focal point to understand the repercussions of challenges during early brain development46. Ultrasonic vocalizations emitted by mouse pups are prominent during the first few days of life and decrease throughout development being almost non-existent at weaning47. These calls are used by rodents to establish and maintain social contacts with conspecifics, and can convey emotional, behavioural and motivational states of the emitter48. Pups USVs also stimulate the innate behaviour from the dams to search for an offspring and retrieve it back to the nest33,49. As observed in other animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders50, we also uncovered deficits in communication at early development stages in mice deficient in GPRASP2. Nonetheless, the communication impairments observed in KO pups arises from a combination of genetic factors and care provided by mutant dams (heterozygous or knockout). With cross-fostering experiments, we could observe that rearing provided by Gprasp2 KO dams was not sufficient to lead to significant alterations in the number of the calls emitted by WT pups. In contrast, KO pups reared by WT dams had the vocalizations phenotype rescued.

Perspectives and significance

The effects of sex-related sociocultural and environmental modifiers, may, to some degree impact the ability of females to be more socially competent at camouflaging autistic symptoms leading to a lack or delay of diagnosis51,52. Nonetheless, sex differences in these disorders are often overlooked, leading to a lack of knowledge and a greater misunderstanding of the impact of ASD-linked mutations in social or maternal behaviour in females. Given the sexual dichotomies present in neurodevelopmental disorders, it is important to understand the communalities and differences when exploring genes linked to these same pathologies in the potential for sex-specific alterations in cognition or behaviour.

The hypothalamus has a prominent role in maintaining the body’s homeostasis, being highly involved in the satiety/feeding regulation, hormone regulation, and also having a crucial role in attachment behaviours and parental care53. The high expression of GPRASP2 in the hypothalamic region, associated with the increase in body weight and the impairments in maternal care/bonding observed in the Gprasp2 KO females suggests a significant role for GPRASP2 in hypothalamic function worth further exploration.

As Gprasp2 is an X-linked gene, to obtain the genotypes required for this study, matings required using Gprasp2 mutant dams. This raises the question of the importance of the genotype of the dam (and the phenotypical alterations associated with it) when caring for offspring and the impact of those same deficits in the development of the pups. If the animals are cared for by a KO dam, would we see an exacerbation of the adult phenotypes in males and females? Or the opposite, if they were fostered by a WT dam, could it be enough to revert the adult phenotypes? These questions may be worth further exploration to understand the role of early social context in overriding or enhancing the consequence of genetic mutations associated with neurodevelopmental disorders.

Study limitations

While the changes to OxtR provide an interesting molecular correlate to the observed behavioural changes, this study does not provide a mechanistic explanation for the perturbed expression of these transcripts. Future studies should address if Gprasp2 is directly involved in the regulation of these transcripts, or rather, if disruption of this gene alters circuit function leading to non-cell autonomous changes in the oxytocinergic system. Additionally, we have not performed a deep characterization of the cross-fostered animals later in adult life. As such, while we observe that early developmental differences are rescued by the cross-fostering experiments, it is unclear if, for example, the pronounce memory defects present in Gprasp2 mutant mice would be equally mitigated.

Conclusions

Our work shows that Gprasp2 deletion led to alterations in female behaviour, including maternal social interactions. These changes impact neurodevelopment and exacerbate alterations in male KO mice in terms of vocalization and delaying weight gain. More generally, this work shows the importance of better understanding the impact of ASD-link mutations in females, and highlights the relevance of controlling for maternal-driven effects and GxE effects in translational studies.